Feeling mental exhaustion? These two areas of the brain may control whether people
Johns Hopkins Medicine
When you're mentally exhausted, your brain might be doing more behind the scenes than you think. In a new study using functional MRI, researchers uncovered two key brain regions that activate when people feel cognitively fatigued regions that appear to weigh the cost of continuing mental effort versus giving up. Surprisingly, participants needed high financial incentives to push through challenging memory tasks, hinting that motivation can override mental fatigue. These insights may pave the way to treating brain fog in disorders like PTSD and depression using brain imaging and behavior-based therapies.
Key Takeaways:
- Two
areas of the brain may work in combination to tell the brain when it's
"feeling" tired.
- People
with depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) often experience
cognitive fatigue.
- Results
of the study may provide a way for physicians to better evaluate and treat
people who experience such fatigue.
In experiments with healthy volunteers undergoing functional
MRI imaging, scientists have found increased activity in two areas of the brain
that work together to react to, and possibly regulate, the brain when it's
"feeling" tired and either quits or continues exerting mental effort.
The experiments, designed to help detect various aspects of
brain fatigue, may provide a way for physicians to better evaluate and treat
people who experience overwhelming mental exhaustion, including those with
depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), the scientists say.
A report on the NIH-funded study was published online June 11 in the Journal of Neuroscience, detailing results on 18 female and 10 male healthy adult volunteers given tasks to exercise their memory.
"Our lab focuses on how [our minds] generate value for
effort," says Vikram Chib, Ph.D., associate professor of biomedical
engineering at the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine and a research
scientist at Kennedy Krieger Institute. "We understand less about the
biology of cognitive tasks, including memory and recall, than we do about
physical tasks, even though both involve a lot of effort." Anecdotally,
Chib says, scientists know cognitive tasks are tiring, and relatively less
about why and how such fatigue develops and plays out in the brain.
The 28 study participants, who ranged in age from 21 to 29,
were paid $50 to participate in the study, and were told they could receive
additional payments based on their performance and choices. All participants
received a baseline MRI scan before the experiments began.
The tests of their working memory, which took place while
undergoing subsequent MRI scans of their brains, included looking at a series
of letters, in sequence, on a screen and recalling the position of certain
letters. The farther back a letter was in the series of letters, the harder it
was to recall its position, increasing the cognitive effort expended. The
participants were given feedback on their performance after each test and
opportunities to receive increasing payments ($1-$8) with more difficult recall
exercises. The participants also were asked before and after each test to
self-rate their level of cognitive fatigue.
Overall, the test results found increased activity and
connectivity in two brain areas when participants reported cognitive fatigue:
the right insula, an area deep in the brain that has been associated with
feelings of fatigue, and the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, areas on both
sides of the brain that control working memory. For each participant, activity
in both brain locations during cognitive fatigue increased by more than twice
the level of baseline measurements taken before starting the tests.
"Our study was designed to induce cognitive fatigue and
see how people's choices to exert effort change when they feel fatigue, as well
as identify locations in the brain where these decisions are made," says
Chib.
Notably, Chib and his research team members Grace Steward
and Vivian Looi found that the financial incentives need to be high in order
for participants to exert increased cognitive effort, suggesting that external
incentives prompt such effort.
"That outcome wasn't entirely surprising, given our
previous work finding the same need for incentives in spurring physical
effort," says Chib.
"The two areas of the brain may be working together to
decide to avoid more cognitive effort unless there are more incentives offered.
However, there may be a discrepancy between perceptions in cognitive fatigue
and what the human brain is actually capable of doing," says Chib.
Fatigue is linked with many neurological conditions,
including PTSD and depression, says Chib. "Now that we've likely
identified some of the neural circuits for cognitive effort in healthy people,
we need to look at how fatigue manifests in the brains of people with these
conditions," he adds.
Chib says it may be possible to use medication or cognitive
behavior therapy to combat cognitive fatigue, and the current study using
decision tasks and functional MRI could be a framework for objectively
classifying cognitive fatigue.
Functional MRI uses blood flow to measure broad areas of
activity in the brain; however, it does not directly measure neuron activation,
nor more subtle nuances in brain activity.
"This study was performed in an MRI scanner and with
very specific cognitive tasks. It will be important to see how these results
generalize to other cognitive effort and real-world tasks," says Chib.
Funding for the research was provided by the National Institutes of Health (R01HD097619, R01MH119086).